Origin and Evolution of Music, Words, and Language.
The evolutionary function of music is language.
Music is Language AND Language is Music.
Interdisciplinary Connections between Language, Music, Evolution, and Reading
Tom Wolfe takes on linguistics
[Language acquisition] [Language and culture] [Language and languages] [Linguistics] [Noam Chomsky]
[Pictorial works] [Pirahã dialect] [Pirahã Indians]
Cover article in Harper's (August, 2016): "The
Origins of Speech: In the beginning was Chomsky".
And so it begins: Nobody in academia had ever witnessed or even heard of a performance like this before. In just
a few years, in the early 1950s, a University of Pennsylvania graduate student — a student, in his twenties —
had taken over an entire field of study, linguistics, and stood it on its head and hardened it from a spongy
so-called “social science” into a real science, a hard science, and put his name on it: Noam Chomsky.
At the time, Chomsky was still finishing his doctoral dissertation for Penn, where he had completed his
graduate-school course work. But at bedtime and in his heart of hearts he was living in Boston as a junior
member of Harvard's Society of Fellows, and creating a Harvard-level name for himself.
EVOLUTIONARY SCIENCE SHOWS THE INTERSPECIES ROOTS OF LANGUAGE
This is how and why we develop PERFECT PITCH
WHY DO CHILDREN SING? -- THE ANSWER
Listen to Human SILBO Whistle
Language
Manuel Carreiras of theand David Corina published research on Silbo in 2004 and 2005 arguing that Silbo was
understood by the brain in much the same way as a spoken language. Their study of speakers of Spanish (some of
whom "spoke" Silbo and some of whom did not) showed (by monitoring brain activity with functional
magnetic resonance imaging) that while non-speakers of Silbo merely processed Silbo as whistling,
speakers of Silbo processed the whistling sounds in the same linguistic centers of the brain that
processed Spanish sentences.
Hear Piraha
sung speech
The Pirahã
The Pirahã habitually whittle nouns down to single syllables.Keren Everett - "The key to learning the language is the tribe's singing, Keren said: the way that the
group can drop consonants and vowels altogether and communicate purely by variations in pitch, stress, and
rhythm — what linguists call “prosody.” I was reminded of an evening in the village when I had heard someone
singing a clutch of haunting notes on a rising, then falling scale. The voice repeated the pattern over and
over, without variation, for more than half an hour.
The Pirahã
habitually whittle nouns down to single syllables. Phonemes (the sounds from which words are constructed) can
feature nasal whines and sharp intakes of breath, and sounds made by popping or flapping the lips. Individual
words were hard to learn, since Also confounding was the tonal nature of the language: the meanings of words
depend on changes in pitch. (The words for “friend” and “enemy” differ only in the pitch of a single syllable.)
Pirahã, like a few other Amazonian tongues, has male and female versions: the women use one fewer consonant than
the men do. Daniel L. (Dan) Everett
I crept up to the edge of one of the Pirahã huts and saw that it was a woman, winding raw
cotton onto a spool, and intoning this extraordinary series of notes that sounded like a muted horn. A toddler
played at her feet. I asked Everett about this, and he said something vague about how tribe members “sing their
dreams.” But when I described the scene to Keren she grew animated and explained that this is how the Pirahã
teach their children to speak. The toddler was absorbing the lesson in prosody through endless repetition—an
example, one might argue, of Edward Sapir's cultural theory of language acquisition at work. “This
language uses prosody much more than any other language I know of,” Keren told me. “It's not the kind of
thing that you can write, and capture, and go back to; you have to watch, and you have to feel it. It's
like someone singing a song
Listen to Piraha sung speech - two boys singing about a day's events. You want to watch and listen and try to sing along with them.
So I started doing that, and I began noticing things that I never transcribed, and things I never picked up
when I listened to a tape of them, and part of it was the performance. So at that point I said, 'Put the
tape recorders and notebooks away, focus on the person, watch them.' They give a lot of things using prosody
that you never would have found otherwise. This has never been documented in any language I know.” Aspects of
Pirahã that had long confounded Keren became clear, she said. “I realized, Oh! That's what the subject-verb
looks like, that's what the pieces of the clause and the time phrase and the object and the other phrases
feel like. That was the beginning of a breakthrough for me. I won't say that I've broken it until I can
creatively use the verbal structure—and I can't do it yet.” Source http://www.newyorker.com/reporting/2007/04/16/070416fa_fact_colapinto?currentPage=all
Update: Keren Everett Has learned to sing.
Hear: WHALE Whistle Hear Humpback Chorus
Hear A Mexican Whistler
BIRDS, BEES, WHALES, DOLPHINS, GORILLA, BONOBO, LION ALL HAVE LANGUAGE.
INTERSPECIES ANIMAL LANGUAGE:
Interspecies Language Evolution From Whistling to Speech. Other species that communicate with each other use
clicks, whistles, song and gestures.
HAND CLAPPING AND HOOTING
You can imagine that the clapping and hooting you hear in the audience is one of the last vestiges of gestures
we still have as evolution propelled us towards a species that became a group to use speech / language.
DRUM LANGUAGE
Ancient Text Messages of the Yoruba Bata Drum - Cracking the Code.
TALKING DRUM - Yoruba speech is coded into a verbal system of vocables that can, in turn, be
translated onto the bata drums. This intermediary coding serves several functions, for instance, providing an
oral notation system that acts as a lingua franca which can be comprehended across geographical regions. While
this coding unites bata musicians, it also establishes, as Villepastour argues, insidership and identity. Most
notably, perhaps, the author highlights the Yoruba practice of using this coded system as a form of spoken
communication in itself, apart from a pedagogical tool for drummers;
The bata is one of the most important and representative percussion traditions of the people in southwest
Nigeria, and is now learnt and performed around the world. In Cuba, their own bata tradition derives from the
Yoruba bata from Africa yet has had far more research attention than its African predecessor. Although the bata
is one of the oldest known Yoruba drumming traditions, the drum and its unique language are now unfamiliar to
many contemporary Yoruba people. Amanda Villepastour provides the first academic study of the bata's
communication technology and the elaborate coded spoken language of bata drummers, which they refer to as
'ena bata'.
Villepastour explains how the bata drummers' speech encoding method links into universal linguistic
properties, unknown to the musicians themselves. The analysis draws the direct links between what is
spoken in Yoruba, how Yoruba is transformed in to the coded language (ena), how ena prescribes the drum
strokes and, finally, how listeners (and which listeners) extract linguistic meaning from what is
drummed. The description and analysis of this unique musical system adds substantially to what
is known about bata drumming specifically, Yoruba drumming generally, speech surrogacy in music and coded
systems of speaking.
DRUM LANGUAGE - AMERICAN VIRGIN
ISLANDS
When Twi is drummed, the resulting drum language is called ayan.
The term ‘2nd line march,' originates in the New Orleans African American/creole traditional funeral processions. The first line in the procession is the family of the deceased, and the band marches along in the second line.
The Etymology of the word
JAZZ is The 2nd Line March beat, as it relates to timing in Irish
tradition starts with the origin of jazz mostly in the New Orleans and the Mississippi River delta.
Western classical music out of Europe usually plays out in simple time (a single time signature and
rhythm) jazz time is rooted in an import from Africa. A Simultaneous juxtaposition and
interaction of two different rhythms, simultaneously. In the case of the 2nd line march beat,
the simultaneous playing of 4/4 and 3 against 2. It's ^two times, at the same time.^
Wynton Marsalis explains "We hear music in one time.
But the Africans, though, they hear music in TWO times! They play in two, AND they play in three… at the same
time." start listening around 4:00 minutes in
then after 7:20, he says: "More difficult, is when we add the four to it." And he has his
buddy clap the same 6/8 rhythm, and Marsalis claps the 4 over the top. "THAT is jig
timing!!"
RHYTHM and ABSOLUTE TIME - WHERE DOES IT COME FROM?The Science of Why Using Music to Teach Children Works.
The Gesture - Before words you will use your hands. Baby Sign Language. The movement of your hand. The sound. Even the way we represent animals is cultural.
The value of play is found in the work of Speech, Clapping, jumping, and circle game rhymes which use the tactile pathways and help wire the brain for higher thinking and reading skills.
National Children's Folksong Repository - the value of children's oral culture - clapping hands and speech.
Structure exposes the evolutionary roots of language * 01 October 2005
Scientists Report Finding a Gene for Speech
How do babies begin to acquire language?
Rhythmic patterns underlie the human language. How children learn the meanings of words. Grammer is hard wired.
Letters are shaped that way for a reason. Rhythm and pitch give the meaning.
By 6 months of age
infants develop a map in the auditory cortex of the phonetic sounds in the native language their mother or
caretaker speaks.
Depending on the language they either will or will not develop perfect pitch.
How do babies begin to acquire language?
"
Much of the information that's transmitted during speech is transmitted by pitch and timing," two of
the crucial elements of music. Rhythmic patterns underlie the human language.
Toddlers Understand Complex Grammar
Children are able to comprehend complex grammar at a younger age. Infants were able to learn two new words in
five minutes with just five presentations for each word and object, said study leader Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, a
professor of psychology at Temple University. Importantly, the babies paired a new word to the object they liked
best, regardless of what object the speaker referred to. Ten-month-olds simply 'glue' a label onto the
most interesting object they see. "The baby naturally assumes that the word you're speaking goes with
the object that they think is interesting, not the object that you show an interest in," Hirsh-Pasek said.
Around 18 months, children learn to use the speaker's interest - such as where the eyes gaze - as a guide to
learning, the researchers say.
Still, Hirsh-Pasek thinks there is a lesson for parents and educators of children at all ages: "Sometimes we fail to take notice of what our learners are doing and what they're interested in," she said. "We all learn best when things are meaningful."
The result is not too surprising, Hirsh-Pasek said in a telephone interview. She says interest drives learning for older children, too, and even adults.
LANGUAGE structure may reveal more about human origins than vocabulary.
The team first looked at a set of 16 Austronesian languages whose histories were already well known via
comparative vocabulary methods. The structural method produced the same historical connections, indicating that
the technique was reliable.
They then analysed a set of 15 Papuan languages that had previously show no historical connections, and
discovered structural similarities (Science, vol 309, p 2072). This suggests the new technique can reach further
back than 10,000 years, says Russell Gray, an expert in language evolution at the University of Auckland, New
Zealand, in an article accompanying the paper.
WHY should links exist between music and language?
SEE HOW THE BRAIN WORKS
Some researchers think that the two might have a common evolutionary origin. Steven Brown, a neuroscientist at
the Karolinska Institute in Huddinge, Sweden, proposes that our ancestors developed a system of communication
that he calls musilanguage, in which meaning was conveyed not so much by the shapes of sounds as by their pitch.
A kind of phrasing akin to the intonation of modern speech could have implied emotive nuances. In support, Brown
points out that some animals make use of pitch to communicate, for example in birdsong, and in the alarm calls
of the African vervet monkey. Brown argues that some remnant of this tone-based musilanguage exists in tonal
languages such as the various forms of Chinese, and in the sing-song of Japanese and Scandinavian languages.
Brown is in good company. Darwin, in his 1871 book The Descent of Man, speculated that language might have
developed from an essentially musical means of communication.
Literacy
Origin of Words, Language Evolution
THE NEW PEDAGOGY AN
INTERDISCIPLINARY MODEL
Integrate Literacy, Music and Technology into the Classroom.
Linguistic Ideology:
Chomsky: The capacity to generate unlimited meaning by placing one thought inside another is the crux of Chomsky's theory—what he calls, quoting the early-nineteenth-century German linguist Wilhelm von Humboldt, “the infinite use of finite means.”
VERSES
Daniel L. (Dan) Everett "immediacy-of-experience principle" a living-in-the-present ethos so powerful that it has affected every aspect of the peoples lives. Committed to an existence in which only observable experience is real, the Pirah do not think, or speak, in abstractionsand thus do not use color terms, quantifiers, numbers, or myths.
VERSES
B. F. Skinner, children learn words and grammar by being praised for correct usage, much as lab animals learn
to push a lever that supplies them with food. In 1959, in a demolishing review of Skinner's book “Verbal
Behavior,” Chomsky wrote that the ability of children to create grammatical sentences that they have never heard
before proves that learning to speak does not depend on imitation, instruction, or rewards. As he put it in his
book “Reflections on Language” (1975), “To come to know a human language would be an extraordinary intellectual
achievement for a creature not specifically designed to accomplish this task.”
Chomsky hypothesized that a specific faculty for language is encoded in the human brain at birth. He described
it as a “language organ,” which is equipped with an immutable set of rules—a universal grammar—that is shared by
all languages, regardless of how different they appear to be. The language organ, Chomsky said, cannot be
dissected in the way that a liver or a heart can, but it can be described through detailed analyses of the
abstract structures underlying language. “By studying the properties of natural languages, their structure,
organization, and use,” Chomsky wrote, “we may hope to gain some understanding of the specific characteristics
of human intelligence. We may hope to learn something about human nature.”Everett conceived his Ph.D.
dissertation at UNICAMP as a strict Chomskyan analysis of Pirahã. Dividing his time between São Paulo and the
Pirahã village, where he collected data, Everett completed his thesis in 1983. Written in Portuguese and later
published as a book in Brazil, “The Pirahã Language and the Theory of Syntax” was a highly technical discussion
replete with Chomskyan tree diagrams. However, Everett says that he was aware that Pirahã contained many
linguistic anomalies that he could not fit into Chomsky's paradigm. “I knew I was leaving out a lot of
stuff,” Everett told me. “But these gaps were unexplainable to me.”
In 1988, Everett was hired by the University of Pittsburgh. By then, Chomsky's system of rules had reached a
state of complexity that even Chomsky found too baroque, and he had begun to formulate a simpler model for the
principles underlying all languages. In his 1921 book, “Language,” Sapir stated that language is an acquired
skill, which “varies as all creative effort varies—not as consciously, perhaps, but nonetheless as truly as do
the religions, the beliefs, the customs, and the arts of different peoples.”Chomsky, however, believed that
culture played little role in the study of language, and that going to far-flung places to record the arcane
babel of near-extinct tongues was a pointless exercise. Chomsky's view had prevailed. Everett began to
wonder if this was an entirely good thing.
“When I went back and read the stuff Sapir wrote in the twenties, I just realized, hey, this really is a
tradition that we lost,” Everett said. “People believe they've actually studied a language when they have
given it a Chomskyan formalism. And you may have given us absolutely no insight whatsoever into that language as
a separate language.”
Everett began to question the first principle of Chomskyan linguistics: that infants could not learn language if
the principles of grammar had not been pre-installed in the brain. Babies are bathed in language from the moment
they acquire the capacity to hear in the womb, Everett reasoned, and parents and caregivers expend great energy
teaching children how to say words and assemble them into sentences—a process that lasts years. Was it really
true that language, as Chomsky asserted, simply “grows like any other body organ”? Everett did not deny the
existence of a biological endowment for language — humans couldn't talk if they did not possess the
requisite neurological architecture to do so. But, convinced that culture plays a far greater role than
Chomsky's theory accounted for, he decided that he needed to “take a radical reëxamination of my whole
approach to the problem.”“Free from Chomskyan constraints, I was able to imagine new relationships between
grammar and culture.” There is a controversial hypothesis advanced
early in the last century by Benjamin Lee Whorf, a student of Sapir's. Whorf argued that the words in our
vocabulary determine how we think. Since the Pirahã do not have words for numbers above two, Gordon wrote,
they have a limited ability to work with quantities greater than that. “It's language affecting thought,”
Gordon told me. His paper, “Numerical Cognition Without Words: Evidence from Amazonia,” was enthusiastically
taken up by a coterie of “neo-Whorfian” linguists around the world.
Michael Tomasello, the director of the Department of Developmental
and Comparative Psychology at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, in Leipzig, endorsed
Everett's conclusions that culture can shape core grammar. Because the Pirahã “talk about different things
[than we do], different things get grammaticalized,” he wrote, adding that “universal grammar was a good try,
and it really was not so implausible at the time it was proposed, but since then we have learned a lot about
many different languages, and they simply do not fit one universal cookie cutter.”
Source http://www.newyorker.com/reporting/2007/04/16/070416fa_fact_colapinto?currentPage=all
Expert Says He Discerns 'Hard-Wired' Grammar
Rules
In 1981 the linguist Noam Chomsky, who had already proposed that language was not learned but innate, made an
even bolder claim.
The grammars of all languages, he said, can be described by a set of universal rules or principles, and the
differences among those grammars are due to a finite set of options that are also innate.
If grammar were bread, then flour and liquid would be the universal rules; the options -- parameters, Dr.
Chomsky called them -- would be things like yeast, eggs, sugar and jalapenos, any of which yield a substantially
different product when added to the universals. The theory would explain why grammars vary only within a narrow
range, despite the tremendous number and diversity of languages.
While most linguists would now agree that language is innate, Dr. Chomsky's ideas about principles and
parameters have remained bitterly controversial.
Even his supporters could not claim to have tested his theory with the really tough cases, the languages
considered most different from those the linguists typically know well.
But in a new book, Dr. Mark C. Baker, a linguist at Rutgers University whose dissertation was supervised by Dr.
Chomsky, says he has discerned the parameters for a remarkably diverse set of languages, especially
American-Indian and African tongues.
In the book, "The Atoms of Language: The Mind's Hidden Rules of Grammar" (Basic Books, 2001), Dr.
Baker sets forth a hierarchy of parameters that sorts them according to their power to affect and potentially
nullify one another.
Just as the periodic table of elements illustrates the discrete units of the physical world, Dr. Baker's
hierarchy charts the finite set of discrete factors that create differences in grammars.
That these parameters can be organized in a logical and systematic way, Dr. Baker says, suggests that there may
be some deeper theory underlying them, and that the hierarchy may even guide language acquisition in
children.
The hierarchy is not the same as a family tree, which illustrates the historical relations among languages --
for example, Italian, French, Spanish and their mother tongue, Latin. Nor does it have anything to do with the
way words vary from language to language. Instead, Dr. Baker analyzes grammar --
the set of principles that describe the order in which words and phrases are strung together, tenses added
and questions formed. Dr. Baker, like Dr. Chomsky, believes these instructions are hard-wired into
humans' brains.
His most spectacular discovery is that the grammars of English and Mohawk, which appear radically different, are
distinguished by just a single powerful parameter whose position at the top of the hierarchy creates an enormous
effect.
Mohawk is a polysynthetic language: its verbs may be long and complicated, made up of many different parts. It
can express in one word what English must express in many words. For example,
"Washakotya'tawitsherahetkvhta'se' " means, " He made the thing that one puts on
one's body ugly for her" -- meaning, he uglified her dress.
In that statement, "hetkv" is the root of the verb "to be ugly." Many of the other bits are
prefixes that specify the pronouns of the subject and object. Every verb includes "each of the main
participants in the event described by the verb," Dr. Baker writes. In all, Mohawk has 58 prefixes, one for
each possible combination of subject, object and indirect object.
Dr. Baker says the polysynthesis parameter is the most fundamental difference that languages can have, and it
cleaves off Mohawk and a few other languages -- for example, Mayali, spoken in northern Australia -- from all
others. That two such far-flung languages operate in the same way is more evidence for the idea that languages
do not simply evolve in a gradual or unconstrained fashion, Dr. Baker says.
At the next junction in the hierarchy, two parameters are at work: "optional polysynthesis" (in which
polysynthetic prefixes are possible, but not required) and "head directionality," which dictates
whether modifiers and other new words are added before or after existing phrases. In English, new words are at
the front. For example, to make a prepositional phrase "with her sister," the preposition goes before
the noun. In Lakota, a Sioux language, the reverse is true. The English sentence "I will put the book on
the table" reads like this in Lakota: "I table the on book the put will."
Japanese, Turkish and Greenlandic are other languages that opt for new words at the end of phrases, while Khmer
and Welsh have the same setting as English.
In all, Dr. Baker and others have identified about 14 parameters, and he believes that there may be 16
more.
Dr. Baker's work is by no means universally accepted. Dr. Robert Van Valin, a professor of linguistics at
the State University of New York at Buffalo, says the findings rest on a questionable assumption: that there is
a universal grammar.
"What they're doing in that whole program is taking English-like structures and putting the words or
parts of words of other languages in those structures and then discovering that they're just like
English," he said.
Dr. Karin E. Michelson, an associate professor of linguistics at SUNY Buffalo, who also disagrees with the
Chomskyan approach, said after reviewing Dr. Baker's Mohawk work that some of the sentences he selected
seemed artificial.
Dr. Baker acknowledged that some of the longer words in his study were "carefully engineered," but he
said the parameter still held up using more common examples of Mohawk. He said using only examples from real
discourse restricted the kind of analysis that linguists could do.
"It would be like constraining a physicist to learn about gravity without ever building a vacuum
tube," Dr. Baker said.
Other linguists, however, say they are excited by Dr. Baker's work. "He's a very influential
linguist, and my guess is that this will provide insights and will spawn research for the next few years,"
said Dr. Stephen Crain, a professor of linguistics at the University of Maryland.
If Dr. Baker's theory is correct, a further question is how the parameters of grammar are set as a child
learns language. Does a child in an English-speaking environment start at the top of the hierarchy,
somehow
grasp that polysynthesis is not at work, and then move on to the next level in the hierarchy?
Dr. Baker also wonders why, if the brain is hard-wired for grammar, it leaves the parameter settings
unspecified. Why aren't they hard-wired, too?
Humans are assumed to have language in the first place because it allows them to communicate useful information
to others. But perhaps, Dr. Baker speculates, language is also a tool of
cryptography -- a way of concealing information from competitors. In that case, he went on, "the
parameters would be the scrambling procedures."